This paper was presented at the 10th Congress of the International Association of Biomedical Gerontology in Cambridge, UK  (September 2003).
This manuscript is now published in the Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 2004, vol. 1019, pp.496-501
[SBN 1-57331-496-X, Item code 0413 ]. 



Early-Life Programming of Aging and Longevity: The Idea of High Initial Damage Load (the HIDL Hypothesis)

Leonid A. Gavrilov and Natalia S. Gavrilova

Center on Aging, NORC and the University of Chicago, 1155 East 60th Street, Chicago, IL 60637-2745, USA

1. Introduction

In 1991 we suggested a scientific idea that early development of living organisms produces an exceptionally high load of initial damage, which is comparable with the amount of subsequent aging-related deterioration accumulating during the rest of the entire adult life [1]. This idea of High Initial Damage Load (the HIDL hypothesis) predicts that even a small progress in optimizing the early-developmental processes can potentially result in a remarkable prevention of many diseases in later life, postponement of aging-related morbidity and mortality, and significant extension of healthy lifespan [1-3]. Thus, the idea of early-life programming of aging and longevity may have important practical implications for developing early-life interventions promoting health and longevity.

In this study we tested the predictions of High Initial Damage Load (HIDL) hypothesis. Specifically, the HIDL hypothesis predicts that early-life events may affect survival in later adult life through the level of initial damage. This prediction is confirmed for such early-life factors as paternal age at person's conception [4] and the month of person's birth [4,5].

Another testable prediction of the HIDL hypothesis is a prevision of an unusual non-linear pattern of lifespan inheritance. This prediction is tested and confirmed -- familial transmission of lifespan from parents to children follows a non-linear (accelerating) pattern with steeper slopes for offspring lifespan of longer-lived parents, as predicted [6].

2. Discussion of the Idea of High Initial Damage Load

An introductory section presented earlier is written as an abstract briefly summarizing the main ideas, findings and conclusions of our studies. The purpose of this section is to provide a more detailed discussion of the idea of high initial damage load.

Reliability theory of aging predicts that a failure rate of simple redundant systems increases with age according to the Weibull (power) law [1-3]. This theoretical prediction is consistent with empirical observations that failure kinetics of technical devices follows the Weibull law [7]. However, biological systems 'prefer' to fail according to the Gompertz (exponential) law [1,8], which calls for explanations.

An attempt to explain exponential deterioration of biosystems in terms of the reliability theory led us to a paradoxical conjecture that biological systems start their adult life with high load of initial damage [1-3].

Although this idea may look like a counter-intuitive assumption, it fits well with many empirical observations on massive cell losses in early development. For example, the female human fetus at age 4-5 months possesses 6-7 million eggs (oocytes). By birth, this number drops to 1-2 million and declines even further. At the start of puberty in normal girls, there are only 0.3-0.5 million eggs - just only 4-8% of initial numbers (see review [3]).

Massive cell losses in early development are creating conditions for Poisson distribution of organisms according to the numbers of remaining cells, which in turn produce the exponential (Gompertzian) law of mortality increase [1]. Because the mathematical proof for this statement is already published elsewhere for a more general case of binomial distribution [1], we can concentrate here on substantive discussion of the idea of high initial damage load in biological systems.

Biological systems are different from technical devices in two aspects. The first fundamental feature of biosystems is that, in contrast to technical (artificial) devices which are constructed out of previously manufactured and tested components, organisms form themselves in ontogenesis through a process of self-assembly out of de novo forming and externally untested elements (cells). The second property of organisms is the extraordinary degree of miniaturization of their components (the microscopic dimensions of cells, as well as the molecular dimensions of information carriers like DNA and RNA), permitting the creation of a huge redundancy in the number of elements. Thus, we can expect that for living organisms, in distinction to many technical (manufactured) devices, the reliability of the system is achieved not by the high initial quality of all the elements but by their huge numbers (redundancy).

The fundamental difference in the manner in which the system is formed (external assembly in the case of technical devices and self-assembly in the case of biosystems) has two important consequences. First, it leads to the macroscopicity of technical devices in comparison with biosystems, since technical devices are assembled 'top-down' with the participation of a macroscopic system (man) and must be suitable for this macroscopic system to use (i.e., commensurate with man). Organisms, on the other hand, are assembled 'bottom-up' from molecules and cells, resulting in an exceptionally high degree of miniaturization of the component parts. Second, since technical devices are assembled under the control of man, the opportunities to pretest components (external quality control) are incomparably greater than in the self-assembly of biosystems. The latter inevitably leads to organisms being 'littered' with a great number of defective elements. As a result, the reliability of technical devices is assured by the high quality of elements, with a strict limit on their numbers because of size and cost limitations, while the reliability of biosystems is assured by an exceptionally high degree of redundancy to overcome the poor quality of some elements.

It follows from this concept of high initial damage load that even small progress in optimizing the processes of ontogenesis and increasing the numbers of initially functional elements can potentially result in a remarkable fall in mortality and a significant improvement in lifespan. This optimistic prediction is supported by experimental evidence of increased offspring lifespan in response to protection of parental germ cells against oxidative damage just by feeding the future parents with antioxidants [9]. Increased lifespan is also observed among the progeny of parents with a low resting respiration rate (proxy for the rate of oxidative damage to DNA of germ cells, see [1]. The concept of high initial damage load also predicts that early life events may affect survival in later adult life through the level of initial damage. This prediction proved to be correct for such early life indicators as parental age at a person's conception [4] and the month of person's birth (see table 1 and earlier publications [4,5]).


FIGURE 1 About here
TABLE 1 About here

Women may be particularly sensitive to early-life exposures, because they are mosaics of two different cell types (one with an active paternal X chromosome and another one with an active maternal X chromosome). The exact pattern of this mosaic is determined early in life. If early-life conditions affect the proportion (or distribution pattern) of cells with a given X chromosome, such conditions might have long-lasting effects in later life. Indeed, this conjecture of stronger female response to early-life exposures is confirmed for such early-life predictors of adult life span as paternal age at a person's conception [4] and the month of a person's birth [4,5].

Another testable prediction of the HIDL hypothesis is a prediction of an unusual nonlinear pattern of life span inheritance. Traditionally, it is assumed that the dependence of progeny life span on parental life span should follow a linear relationship, which is common to all other quantitative traits in classic quantitative genetics [10]. In other words, for each additional year of parental life span, the children are expected to have some fixed gain in their average life span too, as a result of polygenic inheritance of quantitative traits [10]. However, the HIDL hypothesis leads to a very different prediction of a nonlinear (accelerated) "concave-up" pattern of life span inheritance. There should be virtually no life span heritability (a negligible response of progeny life span to the changes in parental life span) when parental life span is below a certain age, and a much higher heritability (an increased response to parental life span) when parents live longer lives. This prediction follows from the hypothesis of HIDL among short-lived parents, whose bodies are damaged during early developmental processes, although their germ cell DNA might be perfectly normal. (If the germ cell DNA were damaged too, these short-lived parents would probably produce offspring who also live short lives. This category will therefore be unlikely to distort the linear dependence of offspring life span on parental life span by a large amount.) Therefore, the progeny of some short-lived parents may have quite normal life spans, well beyond genetic expectations. This result would thus obstruct the classic linear offspring-on-parent dependence for life span. Only at some high parental life span, when most of the germ-normal/somatically damaged parents are eliminated because of their shorter length of life, will the classic linear pattern of life span inheritance eventually reveal itself in its full capacity. This prediction of the HIDL hypothesis was tested and confirmed in humans: Familial transmission of life span from parents to children proved to follow a nonlinear (accelerating) pattern, with steeper slopes for the life span of offspring born to longer-lived parents, as predicted6.

Thus, there is mounting evidence now in support of the idea of fetal origins of adult degenerative diseases, and early-life programming of aging and longevity [4].

Acknowledgments

This study was made possible thanks to a generous support from the National Institute on Aging (NIH, USA), and a stimulating working environment at the Center on Aging, NORC/University of Chicago. We would like to thank members of the Science Advisory Board, SAB (http://www.scienceboard.net/) for useful comments on our work made at the SAB discussion group.

References


  1. Gavrilov, L.A., Gavrilova, N.S., 1991. The Biology of Life Span: A Quantitative Approach, Harwood Academic Publisher, New York.
  2. Gavrilov, L.A. & Gavrilova, N.S. 2001. The reliability theory of aging and longevity. J. Theor. Biol. 213: 527-545.
  3. Gavrilov L.A. & Gavrilova N.S. 2003. The quest for a general theory of aging and longevity. Science's SAGE KE (Science of Aging Knowledge Environment) for 16 July 2003; Vol. 2003, No. 28, 1-10. Available: http://sageke.sciencemag.org
  4. Gavrilov, L.A. & Gavrilova, N.S. 2003. Early-life factors modulating lifespan. In: Rattan, S.I.S. (Ed.). Modulating Aging and Longevity. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 27-50.
  5. Gavrilov, L.A. & Gavrilova, N.S., 1999. Season of birth and human longevity. Journal of Anti-Aging Medicine 2: 365-366.
  6. Gavrilova, N.S. & Gavrilov, L.A. 2001. When does human longevity start? Demarcation of the boundaries for human longevity. Journal of Anti-Aging Medicine, 4: 115-124.
  7. Weibull, W.A. 1951. A statistical distribution function of wide applicability. J. Appl. Mech. 18: 293-297.
  8. Gompertz, B., 1825. On the nature of the function expressive of the law of human mortality and on a new mode of determining life contingencies. Philos.Trans.Roy.Soc.London A, 115: 513-585.
  9. Harman, D., Eddy, D.E., 1979. Free radical theory of aging: beneficial effects of adding antioxidants to the maternal mouse diet on life span of offspring: possible explanation of the sex difference in longevity. AGE 2: 109-122.
  10. Falconer, D.S. & Mackay, T. F. C. 1996. Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. Longman, London.


Table 1. Female lifespan as a function of month-of-birth

Month-of-birth

Net effect*,
in years
(point estimate)

Standard Error

P value

February

0.00

Reference level

March

1.10

0.92

0.2331

April

1.72

0.92

0.0619

May

2.35

0.90

0.0090

June

1.66

0.90

0.0665

July

1.86

0.91

0.0404

August

1.49

0.90

0.0978

September

1.51

0.92

0.0986

October

1.95

0.90

0.0308

November

2.13

0.93

0.0229

December

3.04

0.91

0.0009

January

0.94

0.92

0.3086

February

0.00

Reference level


*Net effect corresponds to additional years of life gained (or lost) compared to the reference category (lifespan for those born in February).

TABLE FOOTNOTE:

Results for Table 1 are obtained through multivariate regression analysis of lifespan data (outcome variable) for 6,908 women born in 1800-1880 (extinct birth cohorts with lifespan known for each person), who survived by age 30 (focus on analysis of adult lifespan). The following additional predictor variables are also included in the final model because of their predictive value: (1) calendar year of birth, (2) ethnicity (Russian, British and others), (3) loss of father during formative years of childhood (before age 15), (4) loss of mother during formative years of childhood (before age 15), (5) cause of death (violent vs non-violent), (6) early death of at least one sibling (before age 30), (7) high birth order (7+), (8) nobility rank of the father (indicator of social status), (9) large family size (number of siblings 9+), (10) maternal lifespan, (11) paternal lifespan, (12) paternal age at person's birth, (13) late paternal age at first childbirth (50+ years), (14) birth of the first child by mother after age 30, (15) death of mother from violent cause of death. The F-value for regression model is 18.12 (p<0.0001).


Figure 1. Daughters' lifespan as a function of paternal age at daughter's birth. 5,063 daughters from European aristocratic families born in 1800-1880. Both parents lived 50+ years. Details of data analysis are described elsewhere [4].


Home

http://longevity-science.org/